Back Transdisciplinarity and the Unity of Knowledge: Beyond the Science and Religion Dialogue


Skip Navigation Links
Home
Agenda
Featured Speakers
Paper Presenters
Public Events
Information
Contact
Registration


Sign up for Conference
2009 Updates.




   

Coralie Buxant
Why People Turn to Religion and Spirituality?: Positive Emotions as Leading to Religion and Spirituality


Abstract

Why people turn to religion and spirituality? As introduction, this paper reviews the considerable cross-sectional, longitudinal, and experimental evidence in psychology of religion literature that shows religion as a refuge from a number of negative psychological experiences and emotions. This evidence gives support to classic theorists who have emphasized the defensive function of religion against frustration, anxiety, fear, deprivation, and so on (e.g., Freud, 1927/1961; Glock, 1964; Marx, 1843/1979). Indeed, religion increases following socioeconomic distress (e.g. Wimberley, 1984). People who have experienced insecurity in attachment to parents or to an adult partner tend to be attracted by religious and spiritual beliefs, practices (Granqvist & Kirkpatrick, in press), and groups (Buxant, Saroglou, Casalfiore, & Christians, 2007; Buxant & Saroglou, 2008). The death of a loved one may also elicit interest in religion and spirituality (e.g., Michael, Crowether, Schmid, & Allen, 2003). Facing an illness, personal crises, and negative life events have often been found to predict conversion and greater religious and spiritual involvement (Spilka, Hood, Hunsberger, & Gorsuch, 2003). More subtly, religion and spirituality seem to buffer against anxiety, especially death-related and existential anxiety. When mortality is made salient, people show stronger belief in God and supernatural agents in general (Norenzayan & Hansen, 2006), and people with extrinsic value orientation show higher levels of spirituality (Cozzolino, Staples, Meyers, & Samboceti, 2004). The religious beliefs of the intrinsically religious people and religious fundamentalists seem to play a protective role in managing terror of death (Friedman & Rholes, 2007, 2008; Jonas & Fischer, 2006).

The present paper asks an alternative point of view: do positive life events and positive emotions can also lead to religion and spirituality? If people may turn to God and spirituality when they lose a loved one, might they also turn to God and spirituality when they fall in love? Similarly, if threats coming from the external world push people to turn to religion and spirituality, can positive emotions relative to this external world also lead to some interest in or increase of religion and spirituality? The aim of the present paper is to investigate whether positive experiences can lead to religion and spirituality.

To our knowledge, it is hard to find empirical evidence that supports a causal link from positive events and emotions to religion (R) or spirituality (SP). There is of course substantial correlational evidence that R/SP, constructs themselves theorized as components of positive psychology (Mattis, 2004; Pargament & Mahoney, 2002), are linked with positivity in life, well-being, happiness, and specific positive emotions (Fredrickson, 2002; Koenig, McCullough, & Larson, 2001; Lewis & Cruise, 2006). However, this causal direction may be double-sided. Positivity in people’s life and the experience of positive emotions are often theorized as being consequences of R/SP (Fredrickson, 2002; Hill, 2002; see also Ciarrocchi & Yanni-Brelsford, 2007; Joseph, Linley, & Maltby, 2006), especially in the presence of previous negative events and emotions, but they may also be, as we argue here, antecedents of attachment to and endorsement of R/SP.

A great deal of theoretical and empirical work on positive emotions provides arguments in favor of our hypothesis that positive experiences and emotions can lead to religion and spirituality. According to Fredrickson’s (1998, 2001) broaden-and-build theory and subsequent research, positive emotions broaden people’s thought-action repertories, encouraging them to discover novel lines of thought and action. For instance, inducing positive affect widens the scope of attention and increases intuition and creativity (see Fredrickson & Losada, 2005, for a review). Studies by Isen and colleagues have shown that when people feel good, their thinking becomes more creative, integrative, flexible and open to information (see Isen, 1987, for review). In addition, positive emotions enhance people’s feeling that life is meaningful (King, Hicks, Krull, & Del Gaiso, 2006) and help them find positive meaning in ordinary events and in adversity (Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000; Fredrickson, 2001). Moreover, positive emotions imply the feeling of “oneness” with other people: they lead to an increase of the self-other overlap and to a more complex understanding of others (Waugh & Fredrickson, 2006), reduce the own-race bias in face recognition (Johnson & Fredrickson, 2005), and lead people to see both ingroup and outgroup members as belonging to one superordinate group (Dovidio, Gaertner, Isen, & Lowrance, 1995). Finally, positive emotions facilitate holistic attentional processing and enhance the individual’s ability to see the “big picture” (Basso, Schefft, Ris, & Dember, 1996; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005).

Based on this research, we hypothesized that positive emotions would make people more open to, interested in, or attached to R/SP. R/SP provides an alternative perception of reality, different from the perception of everyday reality (e.g., Berger, 1997). Religious and spiritual beliefs, rituals, and narratives involve intuition and creativity (Aarnio & Lindeman, 2007; Berry, 1999), positive meaning, belief in the meaningfulness of life, integration and sense of coherence (Park, 2005), positive reframing of negative experiences (Pargament, 1997), as well as prosocial and altruistic ideals and values (Saroglou, Pichon, Trompette, Verschueren, & Dernelle, 2005). More precisely, in the present paper, we make the hypothesis that mere joy, amusement, and pleasure are not sufficient for mobilizing the necessary elements that lead people to embrace a R/SP worldview. However, this should be the case for emotions that imply the experience of marvel, admiration, and the perception of the self as being enveloped by something vaster, greater, which possibly includes an element of mystery. We thus hypothesized that positive attitudes towards religion and spirituality would increase following two specific positive emotional experiences, i.e. a childbirth and appreciation of nature.

This paper presents two studies we conducted to test the causal relation between such positive emotions and R/SP. In both of them, participants (Ns = 91 and 87) watched one of three video clips that primed one of the following emotions: (1) appreciation of nature, (2) wonder at a childbirth, or (3) amusement (comedy); a fourth condition included a control, neutral video. In the first study, we investigated the effect of positive emotions on religiousness. In the second study, we investigated the effect of the same emotions on spirituality.

Going beyond previous studies that have investigated the relationship between positive psychology constructs and religion-spirituality (or the impact of the latter on the former), the present paper provides very initial evidence in favor of a causal link from positive emotions to spirituality. Indeed, results showed that religiousness was somewhat affected by the positive emotions elicited (Study 1, but not Study 2), whereas spirituality was higher among participants who were exposed to the videos eliciting self-transcendent emotions (appreciation of nature and wonder at childbirth) but not among those exposed to humor (Study 2).

Finally, the paper discusses differences between religion and spirituality and suggests that not every kind of positive emotion can elicit spiritual attitudes. Despite limitations of these innovative studies, this paper opens a new vein of research going beyond the negative aspect of psychological (or situational) realities previously studied as leading to religion or spirituality.

References
 

Aarnio, K., & Lindeman, M. (2007). Religious people and paranormal beliefs: Alike or different? Journal of Individual Differences, 28, 1-9.

Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. N. (1996). Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 249-255.

Berger, P. L. (1997). Redeeming laughter: The comic dimension of the human experience. Berlin–New York: Walter de Gruyter.

Berry, C. (1999). Religious traditions as contexts of historical creativity: Patterns of scientific and artistic achievement and their stability. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 1125-1135.

Buxant, C., & Saroglou, V. (2008). Joining and leaving a New Religious Movement: A study of ex-members’ mental health. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture, 11, 251-271.

Buxant, C., Saroglou, V., Casalfiore, S., & Christians, L.-L. (2007). Cognitive and emotional characteristics of New Religious Movements members: New questions and data on the mental health issue. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture, 10, 219-238.

Ciarrocchi, J. W., & Yanni-Brelsford, G. (Eds.). (2007). Special section on positive psychology [Special issue]. Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion, 18.

Cozzolino, P. J., Staples, A. D., Meyers, L. S., & Samboceti, J. (2004). Greed, death, and values: From terror management to transcendence management theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 278-292.

Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Isen, A. M., & Lowrance, R. (1995). Group representation and intergroup bias: Positive affect, similarity, and group size. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 856-865.

Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. American Psychologist, 55, 647-654.

Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300-319.

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden and build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218-226.

Fredrickson, B. L. (2002). Wow does religion benefit health and well-being? Are positive emotions active ingredients? Psychological Inquiry, 13, 209-213.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. A. (2005). Positive emotions broaden scope of attention and thought-action repertories. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313-332.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive affect end the complex dynamics of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60, 678-686.

Freud, S. (1927/1961). The future of an illusion (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York: Norton.

Freud, S. (1928). Humor. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 9, 1-6.

Friedman, M., & Rholes, W. S. (2007). Successfully challenging fundamentalist beliefs results in increased death awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 794-801.

Friedman, M., & Rholes, W. S. (2008). Religious fundamentalism and terror management. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 18, 36-52.

Fuller, R. C. (2006). Wonder: From emotion to spirituality. Chapell Hill, CA: The University of North Carolina Press.

Glock, C. Y. (1964). The role of deprivation in the origin and evolution of religious groups. In R. Lee & M. E. Marty (Eds.), Religion and social conflict (pp. 24-36). New York: Oxford University Press.

Hill, P. C. (2002). Spiritual transformation: Forming the spiritual center of personal energy. Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion, 13, 87-108.

Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes and social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 203-253). New York: Academic Press.

Johnson, K. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2005). “We all look the same to me”: Positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 875-881.

Jonas, E., & Fischer, P. (2006). Terror management and religion: Evidence that intrinsic religiousness mitigates worldview defense following mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 553-567.

King, L. A., Hicks, J. A., Krull, J. L., & Del Gaiso, A. K. (2006). Positive affect and the experience of meaning in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 179-196.

Koenig, H. G., McCullough, M. E., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Handbook of religion and health. New York: Oxford University Press.

Lazarus, R., & Lazarus, B. (1994). Passion and reason: Making sense of our emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.

Lewis, C. A., & Cruise, S. M. (2006). Religion and happiness: Consensus, contradictions, comments and concerns. In S. Joseph, P. A. Linley, & J. Maltby (Eds.), Positive psychology, religion and spirituality [Special issue]. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture, 9, 213-225.

Michael, S. T., Crowether, M. R., Schmid, B., & Allen, R. S. (2003). Widowhood and spirituality: Coping responses to bereavement. Journal of Women and Aging, 15, 145-165.

Marx, K. (1843/1979). Contribution to a critique of Hegel’s Philosophy of Right. In R. C. Tucker (Ed.), The Marx Engels Reader (2nd ed). New York: Norton

Mattis, J. S. (2004). Spirituality. In C. Peterson & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Character strengths and virtues (pp. 599-622). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Norenzayan, A., & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 174-187.

Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religion and coping. New York: Guilford Press.

Pargament, K., & Mahoney, A. (2002). Spirituality: Discovering and conserving the sacred. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 646-659). New York: Oxford University Press.

Park, C. L. (2005). Religion and meaning. In R. F. Paloutzian & C. L. Park (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 295-314). New York: Guilford Press.

Saroglou, V., Pichon, I. Trompette, L., Verschueren, M., & Dernelle, R. (2005). Prosocial behavior and religion: New evidence based on projective measures and peer ratings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 44, 323-348.

Spilka, B., Hood, R. W., Hunsberger, B., & Gorsuch, R. (2003). The psychology of religion: An empirical approach (3d ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

Waugh, C. E. & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Nice to know you: Positive emotions, self-other overlap, and complex understanding in the formation of a new relationship. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 93-106.

Wimberley, D. W. (1984). Socioeconomic deprivation and religious salience: A cognitive behavioral approach. The Sociological Quarterly, 25, 223-238.

Biography

Coralie Buxant received her Ph.D. in psychology from the Université catholique de Louvain, Belgium (2007), where she is currently a visiting lecturer of psychology of religion and a member of the Center for Psychology of Religion (www.psp.ucl.ac.be/psyreli). Moreover, she is lecturer of general psychology at the ICHEC Brussels Management School. She has conducted research on the area of cognitive and emotional determinants of various expressions of religion and spirituality (new religious movements, mainstream conversions, contemporary spiritual seekers) and has co-authored several papers published in international journals (Journal of Positive Psychology, Journal of Religion and Health, Mental Health, Religion and, Culture, Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion, …), as well as a book (Contested Religious Movements: Psychology, Law, and the Politics of Precaution, Gent, Academia). She is the contact person for the Metanexus LSI “Religion, values, and morality in secularized and multicultural societies”.



 

1616 Walnut Street, Suite 1112, Philadelphia, PA 19103 USA  |  Voice: + 1 484.592.0304 Fax: +1 484.592.0313   |   Email  |  Privacy Policy